In antidepressant trials for adults, the mean placebo response rate is 31%, compared with a mean medication response rate of 50%, and it has risen at a rate of 7% per decade over the past 30 years. In children and adolescents with major depression, rates of placebo response are even higher [a mean rate of 46%, compared with a mean medication response rate of 59%], and they have also been increasing over time. High placebo response reduces medication-placebo differences and leads investigators to make methodological modifications [e.g., the use of multiple study sites to increase sample size] that increase measurement error, both of which make it more difficult to demonstrate a statistically significant benefit of a putative antidepressant agent over placebo. Consequently, the average difference between medication and placebo observed in published antidepressant trials has decreased from an average of 6 points on the Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression [HAM-D] in 1982 to 3 points in 2008. For most currently approved antidepressants, fewer than half of the efficacy trials filed with the U.S. Food and Drug Administration for regulatory approval found active drug superior to placebo. While not all trials that fail to distinguish medication from placebo represent false negatives, meta-analyses of antidepressant trials suggest that high placebo response rather than low medication response explains most of the variability in drug-placebo differences. The increasing number of failed trials in recent years has made developing psychiatric medications progressively more time-consuming [average of 13 years to develop a new medication] and expensive [estimates range from $800 million to $3 billion per new agent] compared with medications for non-CNS indications. These considerations contributed to recent decisions by several large pharmaceutical companies to reduce or discontinue research and development on medications for brain disorders, prompting warnings of “psychopharmacology in crisis”.
A Model of Placebo Response in Antidepressant Clinical Trials
by Bret R. Rutherford,M.D. and Steven P. Roose,M.D.
American Journal of Psychiatry. published on-line January 15, 2013.Placebo response in clinical trials of antidepressant medications is substantial and has been increasing. High placebo response rates hamper efforts to detect signals of efficacy for new antidepressant medications, contributing to trial failures and delaying the delivery of new treatments to market. Media reports seize upon increasing placebo response and modest advantages for active drugs as reasons to question the value of antidepressant medication, which may further stigmatize treatments for depression and dissuade patients from accessing mental health care. Conversely, enhancing the factors responsible for placebo response may represent a strategy for improving available treatments for major depressive disorder. A conceptual framework describing the causes of placebo response is needed in order to develop strategies for minimiz- ing placebo response in clinical trials, maximizing placebo response in clinical practice, and talking with depressed patients about the risks and benefits of antidepressant medications. In this review, the authors examine contributors to placebo response in antidepressant clinical trials and propose an explanatory model. Research aimed at reducing placebo response should focus on limiting patient expectancy and the intensity of therapeutic contact in antidepressant clinical trials, while the optimal strategy in clinical practice may be to combine active medication with a presentation and level of therapeutic contact designed to enhance treatment response.
It is intriguing to speculate that natural history factors may be playing a greater role in antidepressant clinical trials over time as the population of patients enrolling in research studies changes. Most research participants in the 1960s and 1970s were recruited from inpatient psychiatric units, whereas current participants are symptomatic volunteers responding to advertisements. Studies are needed to compare the baseline characteristics, treatment response, and attrition rates of self-referred depressed patients with those who respond to advertisements. Natural history factors may be more important in the latter population, with the symptoms experienced by advertisement respondents being more variable and transient, resulting in greater placebo response rates compared with self-referred patients.
The intensive therapeutic contact found in clinical trials may be contrasted with what patients being treated with antidepressants receive in the community. In community samples of patients receiving antidepressant medication, 73.6% are treated exclusively by their general medical provider as opposed to a psychiatrist. Fewer than 20% of patients have a mental health care visit in the first 4 weeks after starting an antidepressant, and fewer than 5% of adults beginning treatment with an antidepressant have as many as seven physician visits in their first 12 weeks on the medication. Thus, assignment to placebo in an antidepressant clinical trial represents an intensive form of clinical management that has therapeutic effects.
Likewise, I find this suggestion bizarre, "Research aimed at reducing placebo response should focus on limiting patient expectancy and the intensity of therapeutic contact in antidepressant clinical trials, while the optimal strategy in clinical practice may be to combine active medication with a presentation and level of therapeutic contact designed to enhance treatment response." I’m afraid I’m too set in my ways to see "therapeutic contact" as a way to augment antidepressant drugs! That’s just too much of a paradigm shift for this old man to take in. And that first part seems kind of strange too – limiting contact and expectations to increase the possibility of seeing a drug effect. I understand the logic, but that means to me that the drug effect is either pretty weak or the subjects in these trials aren’t very sick [or both]. This is hardly a suggestion one would make for a major illness being treated with robust medications. This is a recipe for detecting small differences.
Aside from the obvious problems of diagnosing being unhappy to miserable as MDD and drugging it, did the study look at the use of active placebos in trials?
Come to think about it, does it make any sense at all that there is a brain disease that can be effectively treated with a drug that has no impact on a brain that is not diseased? Or that a person can have a brain disease that is hidden until a psychoactive drug reveals it?
I have a tendency to oversimplify, and I know sample size is only one part of the puzzle, but the more I read about large RCTs, the more my thinking tends to follow Healy’s, (and this post, specifically). If the drug had a dramatic and specific effect, you would probably see it in a very small population. Thus, if two guys drop 50 mics of acid, it won’t take a 40 page assessment instrument, neuroimaging, and a complex algorithm to know that the drug is doing something. We can argue for over half a century about what its doing and whether it has any clinical usefulness, but there are several distinct clinical effects that would be absolutely clear with a very small sample and a very simple assessment instrument. Even if a drug has a subtle and bizarre effect, like SSRIs, I think one may get a better sense of them from a smaller sample size and a population you’re familiar with… Healy’s experiment with SSRIs recruited clinicians he had known for years, and reading about the responses of his colleagues was far more useful than anything I learned in psychopharm (particularly, for example, the fact that very intelligent people frequently do not want to discontinue SSRIs even when they cause akathisia and SI, and tend to blame themselves for SI even when they understand intellectually these responses are often caused by the drugs.)
SSRIs numb.
And feeling more *numb* may make people feel like the drugs are helping – at least at first… but it doesn’t mean the drugs are *working* or providing a *benefit*.
Another reason these drugs *slightly* out-perform placebo is due to the fact that SSRIs have “side effects”… this makes the person feel like *something* is taking place.
When an *active* placebo is used – one with a side effect, such as dry mouth, it’s not uncommon to see the placebo out-perform the SSRI. Studies involve many kinds of manipulation – for example, using a small, white pill for placebo and administering the pill by folks in street clothing vs a white lab coat for the SSRI.
Using a large pill that is colored (pink is the best), and which is marked – with lettering, and a split in the middle changes the results.
The bottom lie is that SSRIs are a scam.
A very *dangerous* scam.
Tomothy Scott, Ph.D. – ‘ The Problem with Drug-Relate Studies’ –
http://www.ihealthtube.com/aspx/viewvideo.aspx?v=337dc5fa25677983
Duane
And they do *not* work for MDD either.
The mantra amongst psychiatrists is to (reluctantly) begin to admit they have “little or no benefit” for mild/moderate depression, but remain quite effective for major depressive disorder.
Really?
Are you kidding me?
How much common sense is there in that proposition?
The drugs can lift 100 lbs of depression, but are unable to lift 25 lbs of depression?
Some of us were born at night.
But none of us were born *last* night.
Duane
Trauma make us feel bad, much shit may make people ill, still more shit can make people psychotic. My experiences have led to the view that these are normal reactions, and that given proper, wholesome care and safety, most may recover… see Richard Bentall on http://www.iai.tv
Exception: Some experiences are too horrible, heartbreaking. Loaded silence passed on – generations of human misery – as sensed around men who survived KZ camps, political prisoners in my familiy, and not the worst off. Their families suffered with them, in spite of psychiatric treatments, drugs, misery, passed on to children. Genes? Sure, normal, human reactions to inhumane, criminal cruelties of war, and to the litany of cruelties possible in peace. Easier, more lucrative to offer drugs than work for change, I think. Modern methods of exploitation..
http://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2012/05/120503142540.htm
Biased Evidence? Researchers Challenge Post-Marketing Drug Trial Practices
“Rigorously designed and executed research has a critical role in improving patient care and restraining ballooning health care costs,” said Kimmelman, associate professor of biomedical ethics at McGill. “There is currently a push to streamline the ethical review of research. In this process, oversight systems should be empowered to separate scientific wheat from marketing chaff.”
Good luck finding any “scientific wheat” with antidepressant research.
Probably more likely to find a needle in a hay stack.
Duane
fyi
Edward Shorter, Ph.D.
The Coming Battle Over DSM-5
A wave of opposition is building against the new edition of psychiatry’s bible.
Published on March 15, 2013 Psychology Today Blog
http://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/how-everyone-became-depressed/201303/the-coming-battle-over-dsm-5-0
Placebo vs Antidepressant –
http://www.ihealthtube.com/aspx/viewvideo.aspx?v=15df4d287254608c
Duane