happy holidays!…

Posted on Friday 25 December 2015

Mickey @ 12:22 AM

a postscript…

Posted on Tuesday 22 December 2015

Well, I can’t seem to let this topic lie. After my last two posts, I ran across a letter to the editor in the American Journal of Psychiatry from Dr. Bernard Carroll, a frequent commenter here, written back in 2009 in response to the meta-analysis by Nelson and Popakostas [Atypical Antipsychotic Augmentation in Major Depressive Disorder: A Meta-Analysis of Placebo-Controlled Randomized Trials]. I’m posting it here with his permission for several reasons. First, he anticipates the cautions expressed in the 2013 meta-analysis by Spielmans et al [Adjunctive Atypical Antipsychotic Treatment for Major Depressive Disorder: A Meta-Analysis of Depression, Quality of Life, and Safety Outcomes] as well as my own concerns about this whole augmentation strategy. In addition, he fleshes out the risk/benefit ratio equation with some actual estimates:

Antipsychotic Drugs for Depression?
Bernard J. Carroll, M.B.B.S., Ph.D., F.R.C.Psych.
American Journal of Psychiatry. 2010 167[2]:216.
To the Editor: The Review and Overview by J. Craig Nelson, M.D., and George I. Papakostas, M.D., published in the September 2009 issue of the Journal, concluded that atypical antipsychotic drugs "are effective augmentation agents in major depressive disorder but are associated with an increased risk of discontinuation due to adverse events". This meta-analysis did not demonstrate effectiveness, only nominal efficacy against placebo. Moreover, the review is unbalanced because risk was not adequately considered.

The authors called the risk of tardive dyskinesia a "rare but serious" concern. Tardive dyskinesia is serious but not rare with atypical antipsychotic drugs. A rate of 8% has been seen in delusional depression. Many cases are persistent, and depressed patients are especially at risk. For aripiprazole, which was the first atypical antipsychotic drug approved for augmentation in depression, a 1.1% rate of dyskinesia within 6 weeks was reported [study #CN138165; ClinicalTrials.gov registry number, NCT00105196]. This translates to 11,000 cases per million exposures. Should we really give this drug to millions of nonpsychotic depressed patients? The marketer’s aggressive media advertising appears to have that aim.

There is insufficient evidence of benefit to offset such risk. The trials of aripiprazole were unblinded by akathisia [25%] and other side effects that can introduce rater bias. Aripiprazole was not efficacious by self-report depression measures, i.e., patients did not find it effective. In one study cited, efficacy was lacking in male subjects. In the meta-analysis, the number needed to treat was nine, signifying only borderline utility in this therapeutic context. The corresponding number needed to treat for lithium is four to five. The larger numbers of patients in the evidence base for atypical antipsychotic drugs, as noted by the authors, reflect the commercial interest in penetrating the depression market, but the manufacturers of these drugs have studiously avoided testing their products against lithium. These concerns about efficacy and relative efficacy were not addressed.

Finally, there is no evidence that augmentation with atypical antipsychotic drugs is useful in the long-term. Drs. Nelson and Papakostas did not note that a study of depressive relapse prevention with risperidone augmentation was negative. No long-term safety or efficacy data have been posted for aripiprazole in the treatment of depression, even though a 52-week study was completed in November 2007 [ClinicalTrials.gov registry number, NCT00095745]. On the basis of the presently available data, psychiatrists should be more forthright in pointing out the limited effectiveness as well as the risks and adverse effects of atypical antipsychotic drugs in the treatment of depression, especially in primary care.

Mickey @ 8:00 AM

extending the risk…

Posted on Monday 21 December 2015

Continued from a worksheet post…

In April 2015, the FDA approved a new Atypical Antipsychotic, Brexpiprazole [Rexulti®]. Around the same time, there were two articles about the Brexpiprazole clinical trials, both released on-line ahead of print in prominent journals [see the spice must flow…]. But there were some odd things about those articles. Each one had only one academic author – the rest were pharmaceutical company employees. But there was more [see anything goes…] – the academic authors were from the same department of psychiatry, the same institute [Feinstein Institute for Medical Research], with similar conflicts of interest, and the medical ghost-writers were also from the same communications firm [QXV Communications, Macclesfield, U.K.]. These were the studies that the FDA evaluated in the approval process. These articles were virtual clones.

Brexpiprazole [Rexulti®] was approved and launched just as Aripiprazole [Abilify®]  went off-patent. And speaking of clones, Aripiprazole and Brexpiprazole sure look like clones to me when I compare their chemical structure:
But that’s not all, folks! Another odd thing, the initial FDA submission covered two indications – and both were approved in April 2015. I’ve never seen that before:
Lundbeck Press Release
September 24, 2014

H. Lundbeck A/S [Lundbeck] and Otsuka Pharmaceutical Co., Ltd. [Otsuka] today announced that the US Food and Drug Administration [FDA] has determined that the New Drug Application [NDA] for brexpiprazole for monotherapy in adult patients with schizophrenia and for adjunctive treatment of major depressive disorder [MDD] in adult patients is sufficiently complete to allow for a substantive review and the NDA is considered filed as of 9 September 2014 [60 days after submission]. The PDUFA date is July 11, 2015…
Then in August 2015, another really odd thing. There were two Brexpiprazole articles by the same authors on the same topic published back to back in the same issue of the same journal [JCP] [with only one academic author each – the same academic author]. This clone thing is getting out of hand!
by Thase ME, Youakim JM, Skuban A, Hobart M, Zhang P, McQuade RD, Nyilas M, Carson WH, Sanchez R, and Eriksson H.
Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2015 76[9]:1232-1240.
by Thase ME, Youakim JM, Skuban A, Hobart M, Augustine C, Zhang P, McQuade RD, Carson WH, Nyilas M, Sanchez R, and Eriksson H.
Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2015 76[9]:1224-31.
That really is a first. Like the two Schizophrenia articles, the augmentation articles were submitted around the same time as the application to the FDA. And as you might have guessed, the ghost-writing firm for both is our friend QXV Communications, Macclesfield, U.K. also. Just one more thing to mention. If you look on clinicaltrials,gov at the trials for Brexpiprazole, this is what you’ll find:
You can’t miss the emphasis on MDD trials for this antipsychotic drug [meant for Schizophrenia]. And as for the odd stuff, they don’t seem to mind that the experommercial nature of these articles [industry funded, industry run, industry written] is blatantly obvious, or that it’s crystal clear that the market they’re aiming for is the augmentation-of-treatment-resistant-depression market, or even that Brexpiprazole is an obvious patent extender clone for the wildly successful Abilify®. I expect they’re going to sell it as the hot new drug by detailing doctors, and even here in the woods of Appalachia, we’re already seeing people [non-psychotic people I might add] showing up taking it.

I’m not in love with this story about the marketing practices of big PHARMA, but that’s not really any of my business. But there’s something else that is. When I first started working as a volunteer in the clinics up here, it seemed like everyone was taking Seroquel®. I couldn’t figure out why. At the time, it was the number one selling drug in the country. That’s actually what got me blogging here in the first place. Over time I finally got every non-psychotic patient off of the Seroquel® – worried about Tardive Dyskinesia and the metabolic syndrome, particularly with long term use. I was kind of proud about getting them off that drug. Recently, our clinic has expanded its scope, so there has been an influx of new patients, at least new to me, and guess what? – a lot of them are on Atypical Antipsychotics [not just Seroquel®, but Abilify® and the others]. But here’s the bad part, I’ve got three of those new cases who have definite early Tardive Dyskinesia [a lot for my small practice]. Mercifully, two seem to be waning now they’re off of the drug [fingers crossed], but I’m afraid one is probably around forever. They’ve all been on the drugs for a long time [which is what happens these days]. And I doubt such symptoms happen in short trials.

The Editorial in December’s American Journal of Psychiatry – Adjunctive Ziprasidone in Major Depression and the Current Status of Adjunctive Atypical Antipsychotics  by J. Craig Nelson, M.D. who is the same guy who coauthored the very first augmentation study and has subsequently written extensively on the subject [see a worksheet post…] – ends with this:
Although the acute efficacy of the adjunctive atypical antipsychotics is well established, the major shortcoming is the dearth of long-term placebo-controlled studies that would inform clinicians about persistence of efficacy or burden of adverse effects. Only two studies have been performed. In one, risperidone failed to have a significant effect in preventing relapse; however, methodological problems may have contributed to the result. A recent 26-week relapse prevention study of the olanzapine-fluoxetine combination showed a significant advantage for the combination. In that trial, after 6–8 weeks of acute treatment with olanzapine and fluoxetine and 12 weeks of stabilization, patients were randomly assigned to the combination or to fluoxetine plus placebo. A time-to-relapse analysis significantly favored the combination. Under the primary definition of relapse, 15.8% of the patients on the combination relapsed, compared with 31.8% of the patients on fluoxetine. Nevertheless, the paucity of long-term controlled studies for the atypical agents limits our understanding of the persistence of efficacy and of side effects that may increase or emerge with time. This leaves the clinician whose patient has responded to acute adjunctive treatment with little information for weighing the risks and benefits of continuing the adjunctive agent.
Good for him for saying that part at the end. But I know the answer. Once people start these drugs, they just keep taking them and nobody takes them off. My cases of Tardive Dyskinesia are a testimonial to what can happen – a tragic testimonial.

So that’s why I liked the Spielmans et al meta-analysis [the extra mile…]. It showed us that from the patients’ own ratings, they weren’t getting that much benefit – a very important piece of data. And for that matter, Spielmans et al documented the Adverse Events including the high incidence of Akasthisia [a Tardive Dyskinesia harbinger] with Abilify®. So I know why I’ve been so stuck on this business of Atypical Antipsychotic augmentation of the so-called treatment-resistant-depression. All of these Atypicals may have a low moderate effect on TRD symptoms, sure enough, but these drugs are time-bombs [particularly] if they are used long term [and they are used long-term!]. So if nobody’s really looked long term in the last 16 years, Why the hell not?

They’re not just using Brexpiprazole to extend the patent of Abilify®. They’re  extending the risk in the process – a big risk that’s been largely ignored…

Note: The only long term Incidence of TD study I know of is the 2010 European Schizophrenia Study [sponsored by Eli Lilly]…
Mickey @ 12:56 PM

a worksheet post…

Posted on Sunday 20 December 2015

In the 1970s, no psychiatrist worth their salt couldn’t draw the NIMH Memorial Neuron and sketch in the receptors and re·uptake pathways. Back then, it was catecholamines, dopamine, but later came serotonin. I haven’t seen anybody do that for years. I guess people fled when confronted about the phrase chemical imbalance and other bio·hypotheses that were only speculations. But I was an Internist first, and my head is still filled with the drawings of the Krebs Cycle, Nephrons, chemical pathways, anatomy, etc etc. It was the way I learned and they just don’t wear away with time. One of the main things I taught in the Psychoanalytic Institute was what we call Object Relations Theory – and those diagrams are still in there too. I’m a visual learner I guess, and I love me a diagram.

So when I got on my current kick of looking into augmenting SSRI failures with Atypical Antipsychotics, in spite of my skepticism, I wondered "What mechanism they proposed?" and "Who thought up trying it the first time?" Usually, the answers to such questions just come up when I’m darting from article to article – but not this time.  So I actually wrote some people. A few laughed and said, "Rationale? They don’t need a Rationale," one adding, "there were drugs to be sold!" Most simply said something like, "No clue." Nobody seemed to know.

I had no idea why it mattered to me, but it stayed in my mind looking at the articles when I was going through the various meta-analyses. I finally got a clue from an unusual resource – a [likely ghost-written] 2005 review article by Dr. Charlie Nemeroff:
by Charles B. Nemeroff, M.D., Ph.D.
Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2005 66 [suppl 8].

Treatment options for bipolar depression and treatment-resistant unipolar depression include augmentation of antidepressant therapy with a nonantidepressant drug, including atypical antipsychotics. Risperidone is effective in combination with fluvoxamine, paroxetine, or citalopram in treatment-resistant unipolar depression, with reported remission rates of 61% to 76%. Olanzapine in combination with fluoxetine is safe and effective in patients with bipolar depression and those with fluoxetine-resistant unipolar depression. Ziprasidone and aripiprazole augmentation of various selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors has been reported to be effective in refractory unipolar depression in open-label studies. Data on use of quetiapine or clozapine as augmentation therapy for depression or anxiety are not yet available. Further double-blind, placebo-controlled studies of augmentation of antidepressants with atypical antipsychotics in refractory depression and anxiety are justified based on the available literature.
He [or his writer] said:
Support for investigation of atypical antipsychotics in patients with depression comes partly from preclinical studies suggesting that several atypical antipsychotics are potent 5-HT2A antagonists at low doses31–33 and may facilitate the action of serotonin at the 5-HT1A receptor, thereby augmenting the efficacy of SSRIs24
It was that little 24 that finally lead me to Ground Zero:
by Robert B. Ostroff, M.D., and J. Craig Nelson, M.D.
Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 1999 60:256–259.

Background: At low doses, risperidone acts as a 5-HT 2 antagonist. Preclinical data suggest 5-HT 2 antagonists may enhance the action of serotonin. This report examines the clinical use of risperidone to augment selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor [SSRI] antidepressants in patients who have not responded to SSRI therapy.
Method: In 8 patients with major depressive disorder without psychotic features [DSM-IV] who had not responded to an SSRI, risperidone was added to the ongoing SSRI treatment. Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression scores were obtained before and after the addition of risperidone.
Results: These 8 patients remitted within 1 week of the addition of risperidone. Risperidone also appeared to have beneficial effects on sleep disturbance and sexual dysfunction.
Conclusion: Risperidone may be a useful adjunct to SSRIs in the treatment of depression.
… and here’s the rationale:
"The current article describes the use of risperidone as an augmentation strategy. Both preclinical and clinical data provide suggestive evidence that augmentation with risperidone might be effective in treating depression. Risperidone is an atypical antipsychotic, which, at low doses, is about 100 times more potent in antagonizing the 5-HT 2A receptor than the D 2 receptor. The 5-HT 2A receptor is an excitatory receptor that acts in opposition to the postsynaptic 5-HT 1A receptor; thus, antagonism of the 5-HT 2A may facilitate the action of serotonin at the 5-HT 1A receptor. This was demonstrated by a preclinical study [in rats, in 1985] which found that ketanserin, a 5-HT 2 antagonist, enhanced the inhibitory effects of serotonin on prefrontal neurons. This preclinical finding suggests that addition of a 5-HT 2 antagonist to a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor [SSRI] might augment the effects of the SSRI."
That 1985 preclinical rat study was publicly funded basic science research:
by Lakoski JM, Aghajanian GK.
Neuropharmacology. 1985 24:265–273.

The ability of the putative serotonin2 [5-HT2] antagonist ketanserin, to alter serotonin [5-HT]-induced responses in cell firing was examined in the prefrontal cortex, the lateral geniculate nucleus and the dorsal raphe nucleus of the rat by microiontophoretic extracellular single unit recording techniques. In the prefrontal cortex, ketanserin failed to antagonize the inhibitory effects of 5-HT recorded in cerveau isolé or preparations anesthetized with chloral hydrate [pure excitatory responses to 5-HT were not observed in either of these preparations]. Paradoxically, the inhibitory response produced by 5-HT [but not gamma-aminobutyric acid, tryptamine or norepinephrine] was potentiated, even in cells where ketanserin alone did not alter spontaneous firing rates. The systemic administration of ketanserin [5 mg/kg, i.p.] had effects similar to those observed in the microiontophoretic experiments in the prefrontal cortex. In the dorsal raphe nucleus of animals anesthetized with chloral hydrate, ketanserin neither attenuated nor potentiated the inhibition of serotonergic neurons by 5-HT. In the lateral geniculate nucleus, as in the prefrontal cortex, ketanserin potentiated rather than attenuated, the inhibitory effect of 5-HT. Ketanserin was found to attenuate the excitatory responses produced by norepinephrine, an alpha 1-adrenoceptor-mediated response, in the lateral geniculate nucleus. The observed potentiation by ketanserin of inhibitory responses to 5-HT but not those of gamma-aminobutyric acid, tryptamine or norepinephrine, recorded in the prefrontal cortex, may be consistent with the proposed interaction between ketanserin and a specific 5-HT2 binding site.
It  was done at Yale in 1985, where Drs. Ostroff and Nelson were in 1999 – a long time later [1999 – 1985 = 14 years]. I spent some time chasing it down, but once I located it, I found myself doubting that explanation. It sounded to me more serendipitous – like some doctor had a really depressed patient and said, "Why not?," gave some Risperidone, and it helped. Then they did their little series above, and they were off and running.  The other possibility that I entertained was that Janssen thought of it. That was around the time when Janssen was pushing Risperidone for any and everything. But I can’t find away way to follow those thoughts.

One thing did sort of leap out. Dr. J. Craig Nelson who co-authored that first trial in 1999 was the same person that wrote the recent AJP Editorial that got me going down this road in the first place [creative funding I…] now sixteen years later. In addition, I found 12 other augmentation articles of his in pubMed by searching (nelson j craig[Author]) AND augmentation – several openly promoting Aripiprazole [Abilify®] [an example, Augmentation treatment in major depressive disorder: focus on aripiprazole – be sure to look at the Disclosures].

In the course of putting my ramblings on paper then taking a break, I’ve figured out why I’ve been so stuck on these Atypical Antipsychotic trials in depression. Why did I get so focused? Why did they even try them in the first place? Why was I obsessing on the details? Because that’s where the money is. One of the witnesses in the TMAP trial said, "You can’t make a blockbuster drug [>$1B] out of a 1% disease [Schizophrenia]." And this is what he meant. This augmentation indication is where the profits are. There are tons of people taking antidepressants who are dissatisfied with the results, and that’s where these drugs come into play. Compared to Schizophrenia, that’s a huge market – so that’s where the ads have been aimed, the articles focused:
Usually, when I write one of these posts, I know where I’m headed, what I want to say. But this time, I didn’t. I just knew the topic, one I can’t seem to let go of. I  wrote the part above the horizontal line, then headed off to an annual holiday party in Atlanta with my former practice partners and good friends. They asked a lot about my 329 paper and this blogging thing I do, because it’s not what I did back before I retired in 2003. Then, when I got home last night and read what I’d written, I saw it in an entirely different light. So I’m going to consider this post just the worksheet it is, and start over…
Mickey @ 10:02 AM

the extra mile…

Posted on Saturday 19 December 2015

The topic here is augmentation with Atypical Antipsychotics in treatment resistant depression [once again], but it’s also about meta-analyses in general. In my recent in the land of sometimes[3], I was looking at the techniques used in making the forest plots now widely used in meta-analyses. It came to mind as I was comparing these two examples [essentially covering the same ground]:
by J. Craig Nelson, M.D. and George I. Papakostas, M.D.
American Journal of Psychiatry. 2009 166:980-991.

Objective: The authors sought to determine by meta-analysis the efficacy and tolerability of adjunctive atypical antipsychotic agents in major depressive disorder.
Conclusions: Atypical antipsychotics are effective augmentation agents in major depressive disorder but are associated with an increased risk of discontinuation due to adverse events.
In creative funding III & some other things… where I reviewed part of it. I said, "It’s a decent article, well researched with lots of useful information" but I could’ve added, "but there’s a much more comprehensive one coming." Alto mentioned this next one in the comments, and it answered some questions I hadn’t gotten around to asking:
by Glen I. Spielmans, Margit I. Berman, Eftihia Linardatos, Nicholas Z. Rosenlicht, Angela Perry, and Alexander C. Tsai
PLoS Medicine. 2013 10[3]:e1001403.

Background: Atypical antipsychotic medications are widely prescribed for the adjunctive treatment of depression, yet their total risk–benefit profile is not well understood. We thus conducted a systematic review of the efficacy and safety profiles of atypical antipsychotic medications used for the adjunctive treatment of depression.
Conclusions: Atypical antipsychotic medications for the adjunctive treatment of depression are efficacious in reducing observer-rated depressive symptoms, but clinicians should interpret these findings cautiously in light of [1] the small-to-moderate-sized benefits, [2] the lack of benefit with regards to quality of life or functional impairment, and [3] the abundant evidence of potential treatment-related harm.
The 2009 paper was essentially a collection of forest plots: Response Rate, Remission Rate, All cause Discontinuation Rate, Discontinuation Rate due to Adverse Events, in several groupings – all using Dichotomous [categorical, yes/no] Outcome Variables. Recall from in the land of sometimes[3] that with categorical variables, the criteria used matters, and in this case, the criteria for Remission varied among studies. In addition, categorical variables are derived. Continuous variables are independent of criteria, universally measured, and closer to the raw data. Spielmans et al provided the resulting Hedges g Effect Sizes in a table, so I put them into forest plots. First, here are the clinician rated metrics
…mirroring the finding in Nelson et al of a moderate effect from the augmentation on Depression [MADRS, HAM-D] and Global Inventory. But Spielmans et al did something else. They looked at the [frequently ignored] self-report measures:
The only continuous self-report measure of depression used in these trials was the Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology Self Report. Continuous measures of quality of life included the Quality of Life Enjoyment and Satisfaction Questionnaire [Q-LES-Q] and the Short Form 36 Health Survey [SF-36]. The only continuous measure of functional impairment employed in these trials was the Sheehan Disability Scale [SDS]…
These self-report measures are often given short shrift in articles. But if you think about it, they should be front and center. When I write a prescription, the next meeting usually starts with a report about the effect of the drug – efficacy and side effects. I recently joked about my patients’ "fingers thing" as an outcome measure [see  an aside…], but I was also dead serious.
In the clinical setting, a medicine either helps the patient or it doesn’t, and that’s the whole reason for giving it. So I made a forest plot of the self-report measures Effect Sizes too…
… that was certainly informative. Except for Risperidone, these measures were weak to nil. And they looked further. The asterix indicates the results after removing cases with protocol violations for Ariprazole. But when they looked at the Risperidone study, there was something else:
…The effects of risperidone may have been exaggerated by the reliance on post hoc analysis rather than a priori analysis in the largest study of the drug, as the effect of the drug was greater at 6 wk (g= 0.46) than at the prespecified primary end point of 4 wk [g= 0.32].
…The effect of aripiprazole on quality of life/functioning should be interpreted with caution, as the effect for the drug on the SDS was very small and no longer statistically significant when patients who violated study protocol were excluded from analysis [g=0.12, p=0.08]. Similarly, the effect of risperidone on quality of life/functioning should be interpreted tentatively since it is largely driven by post hoc analyses.
One important thing to keep in mind about meta-analyses, for all the clarity they bring by including a range of studies, they also hide the details and context of any given study by simply reporting "the numbers." And this is such a case. Most of the depression data and all of the SDS and Q-LES-Q numbers for Risperidone come from a 2007 Janssen report by Mahmoud et al – an all Janssen employees article with a tainted history [see ANTIPSYCHOTIC DRUGS FOR DEPRESSION?, trembling earth, mirrored water…, racketeer influenced and corrupt organizations…, optional reading…, etc]. This was a darkest hour that involved a 2005 review article on this topic by Dr. Nemeroff [Use of Atypical Antipsychotics in Refractory Depression and Anxiety]; a 2006 article by Rapaport, Keller, Nemeroff, Mahmoud and other Janssen employees initially claiming efficacy for Risperidone augmentation, but later declared negative by the first author [see Effects of risperidone augmentation in patients with treatment-resistant depression: Results of open-label treatment followed by double-blind continuation, Response by Dr. Bernard Carroll, Author’s Reply]; and much much more. So I would say that Spielmans et al’s caution to "interpret… tentatively" the Risperidone data is an understatement at best. They might just as well substitute "ignore altogether."

There’s nothing particularly wrong with the 2009 Nelson and Papakostas meta-analysis. They reported "the numbers" accurately. But we owe a debt of gratitude to Spielmans et al for going the extra mile and looking critically at the nuances of the individual studies involved [many more findings than mentioned here]. They make the risk/benefit equation come alive for the clinicians who actually use these drugs and the patients who take them…

UPDATE: I failed to mention that Spielmans et al also compiled those Adverse Events with significant Effect Sizes [p<0.10] shown below [truncated]:

Mickey @ 7:35 AM

and throw away the key…

Posted on Thursday 17 December 2015

Mickey @ 4:07 PM

in the land of sometimes[4]

Posted on Thursday 17 December 2015


by Davis CE.
American Journal of Epidemiology. 1976 104[5]:493-498.

"Regression to the mean is the phrase used to identify the phenomenon that a variable that is extreme on its first measurement will tend to be closer to the center of the distribution for a later measurement. In studies based on biological measurements, this variability can be attributed to both the inherent variation in the phenomenon being measured and the variability of the measurement itself. The concept of regression to the mean is an important consideration in studies where subjects are chosen because of a biological variable above or below a specified level…
This is a phenomenon that has a lot to do with the clinical trials that we talk about all the time. Yet when it comes up, I often feel something like shame because invariably I realize that I’ve forgotten that it even exists, much less what it means precisely, or the why? of it. So this post is an exploration of not the why? of regression to the mean, but the why? it’s so forgettable.

It’s counter-intuitive. It means if you give a metric like the HAM-D to a group, and then pick out one subject who scores in the shaded area [above a set cut-off value], the subsequent value from that subject will tend to be lower – moving down towards the mean. Likewise, a subject with a score at the lower end’s next value will tend to be higher – again moving up towards the mean. Why don’t they stay the same? Perhaps that’s why regression to the mean is so hard to hold onto, because it’s the essence of what’s different about statistics and our everyday dealings with numbers. We’re working with a trend rather than a certainty, with values that move when no operation has been applied except resampling. And since the speed with which repeated samples tend to creep towards the mean slows down as it gets closer, it tends to to create a graph that looks familiar [often misinterpreted]. And when we go hunting for the reasons, we find explanations like this…

… and maybe that’s another reason it doesn’t stick – who’s going to carry that around in their mind? Apparently, not me. Probably you won’t either [if you even read it]. So since what we’re aiming for isn’t the why? of regression to the mean, but the why? it won’t stay in mind, maybe we should come at it from a different angle – base it on something we already know. We all seem to accept that if we repeatedly sample something in nature, we’ll get a range of answers, and if we look at the frequency in that range, it’ll look like the curve on the right. Then we can find a Mean [μ] that we accept as the true value [though they’re really all true]. And we can find a Standard Deviation [σ] that represents the variability. We even call it the normal distribution – a testimony to the fact that’s it’s what we normally expect to find in nature.

If you think about it, the shape of the normal distribution is nothing more than an example of regression to the mean. Our repeated samples move towards and collect around the Mean [μ] as if pulled there by some invisible force – a gravity [but as Einstein pointed out about Newton’s force of gravity, it’s not really a force – it’s just in the nature of things]. So regression to the mean is simply a part of sampling nature. And why? should we expect that some extreme value would do anything but trend towards the mean of the distribution it’s part of? Duh! It’s trying to go towards home or maybe home is calling [if you go for anthropomorphic metaphors].

Probably a better way to think about why? we forget about regression to the mean, or why? we expect that some extreme value would do anything but migrate toward the mean, would be to think about our thinker. We invented a mathematics of arithmetic and algebra to fit our minds, so we’re the exceptions. And we try to pull nature into the way our minds work. It’s a bit like our building things based on straight lines and right angles, whereas nature builds with an infinite series of curves [the beauty of nature we so admire]. What we call statistics is our attempt to see things as distributions rather than singularities, as trends rather than certainties, and it’s often hard going because the mind keeps trying to pull things back into our right-angle-straight-line frameworks [more anthropomorphism]. But what I call the land of sometimes is actually closer to the real world of nature than our precision mathematics.

Back to regression to the mean. It’s better to remember the what? than to try to hold on to the why? [because there really isn’t a why?]. Does this matter? or are these just the ramblings of an old man? or both? Well it matters a whole lot in this world of clinical drug trials for sure. And it has much to do with the ways and means of distorting  their results [a major topic of this blog]. But I’m going to let it lie for a while in fear of becoming a disappearing bookmark in your browser. If you want to read more, here are some sites that are more eloquent than I about this whole business [including the finder’s original article]:
Mickey @ 12:57 PM

[again]…

Posted on Wednesday 16 December 2015


Clinical Outcome After Antipsychotic Treatment Discontinuation in Functionally Recovered First-Episode Nonaffective Psychosis Individuals: A 3-Year Naturalistic Follow-Up Study
by Jacqueline Mayoral-van Son, MD; Victor Ortiz-Garcia de la Foz, VTE; Obdulia Martinez-Garcia, PhD; Teresa Moreno, MD; Maria Parrilla-Escobar, MD; Elsa M. Valdizan, MD, PhDc; and Benedicto Crespo-Facorro, MD, PhD
Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. Published online 12/08/2015

Objective: The timing of antipsychotic discontinuation in patients who have fully recovered from their initial episode of psychosis is still open to discussion. We aimed to evaluate the risk of symptom recurrence during the 3 years after antipsychotic discontinuation in a sample of functionally recovered first-episode nonaffective psychosis [FEP] patients [DSM-IV criteria] with schizophrenia spectrum disorder.
Method: Participants in this open-label, nonrandomized, prospective study were drawn from an ongoing longitudinal intervention program of FEP from a university hospital setting in Spain. From July 2004 to February 2011, functionally recovered FEP individuals were eligible if they met the inclusion criteria of [1] a minimum of 18 months on antipsychotic treatment, [2] clinical remission for at least 12 months, [3] functional recovery for at least 6 months, and [4] stabilization at the lowest effective doses for at least 3 months. Forty-six individuals who were willing to discontinue medication were included in the discontinuation group [target group]. Twenty-two individuals opted to stay on the prescribed antipsychotic medication and therefore were included in the maintenance group [control group]. Primary outcome measures were relapse rate at 18 and 36 months and time to relapse.
Results: The rates of relapse over the 3-year period were 67.4% [31 of 46] in the discontinuation group and 31.8% [7 of 22] in the maintenance group. The mean time to relapse was 209 [median = 122] days and 608 [median = 607] days, respectively [log rank = 10.106, P = .001]. The resumption of antipsychotic medication after the relapse occurred was associated with clinical stability and lack of further relapses. When the overall group of relapsed individuals from the 2 conditions [N = 38] was compared to those who remained asymptomatic after 3 years [N = 30], there were significant differences [P < .05] in total scores on the Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms, the Clinical Global Impressions scale, and the Disability Assessment Schedule.
Conclusions: Antipsychotic treatment discontinuation in individuals who had accomplished a functional recovery after a single psychotic episode was associated with a high risk of symptom recurrence. Relapsed individuals had a greater severity of symptoms and lower functional status after 3 years.

Trial Registration: ClinicalTrials.gov identifier: NCT02220504
Author contributors: Drs Mayoral-van Son, Parrilla-Escobar, and Moreno collected clinical data. Dr Mayoral-van Son interpreted the results and drafted the manuscript. Mr Ortiz-Garcia de la Foz performed the statistical analyses and drafted the manuscript. Dr Crespo-Facorro interpreted the results and reviewed the manuscript. Drs Valdizan and Martinez-Garcia helped in the interpretation of clinical data and reviewed the manuscript. The corresponding author had access to all study data. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript.
Potential conflicts of interest: Dr Crespo-Facorro has received honoraria for his participation as a speaker at educational events from Otsuka, Lundbeck, and Johnson & Johnson. Drs Mayoral-van Son, Valdizan, Parrilla, Moreno, and Martinez-Garcia and Mr Ortiz-Garcia de la Foz report no additional financial or other relationship relevant to the subject of this article.
Funding/support: The present study was conducted at the Hospital Marqués de Valdecilla, University of Cantabria, Santander, Spain, under the following grant support: SENY Fundación Research Grant CI 2005-0308007 and Fundación Marqués de Valdecilla API07/011.

The issue of maintenance antipsychotics following the recovery from the first episode of psychosis is surrounded by a layer of controversy overdetermined by guild, commercial, and ideological biases that pervade the interpretation of almost everything said or written about it. This study from Spain just out will surely be looked at through those same lenses that make things so hard to evaluate. Since it will surely be compared to the Wunderink et al study [see persistence…], I thought I’d post them side by side…


… and then again in a scaled-and-colored-to-match version to show the difference in parallel. For review, Wunderink et al’s initial report after two years found what has traditionally been reported – that maintenance medication had a significant positive effect on relapse rates. However, in a later follow-up, they found that the relapse rates equalized [from about three years on], and on functional testing, the patients who were not on a fixed maintenance schedule actually came out better by a respectable margin at about seven years. This new study from Spain did not replicate Wunderink’s findings:

A systematic review of studies exploring the recurrence of psychotic symptoms with antipsychotic discontinuation in first-episode nonaffective psychosis reported a weighted mean recurrence rate of 77% during the first year and more than 90% at 2 years.33 In contrast, the 1-year risk of recurrence within the medication continuation group was estimated at 3%. These estimates differed considerably from the 1-year rates of relapse in FEP patients of 61% in the discontinuation group and 26% in the continuation medication group reported by Leucht and colleagues.1 Methodological discrepancies in the study design; inclusion criteria; and duration of follow-up, diagnosis, and hospitalization rates make comparability between studies problematic. Wunderink and colleagues, exploring remitted psychotic patients, observed that relapse rates were 2 times higher in individuals who gradually tapered or discontinued medication, and that only 20% of patients could be successfully discontinued. Strikingly, those patients with an earlier reduction or discontinuation treatment strategy appear to have long-term functional gains compared with individuals who maintained treatment. Stopping antipsychotic medication has been repeatedly demonstrated as the biggest predictor of relapse in schizophrenia. It is of note that, despite most relapsed patients’ responding promptly to resuming antipsychotics, our data herein indicate that patients who suffered a relapse had a decreased functional status and a greater severity of symptomatology at 3 years compared with those patients who did not relapse.

There are some real differences in the way these two studies were conducted, but the goal of the research was the same, and their take home messages were strikingly different. This study was independently funded and conducted in a single university hospital setting, getting around some of the concerns that confound this kind of clinical trial. Like most of the European Social Democracies, Spain has a National Health Service and it’s ranked as one of the more effective. As best I can tell, this was a well-conducted study. It certainly mirrors my own [admittedly limited] experience.

But the general tone of the discussions about maintenance medications in psychotic illness has been so contentious for so long that it would be naive to think that any one study will bring things to any resolution. The dialog has been too divisive to expect anything to be that simple. The issues of maintenance antipsychotics, involuntary hospitalization, forced drugging, commercialization, conflicts of interest, the medical model, guild vs guild, the recovery and survivor movements, etc. have gotten all glommed up together and made productive dialog difficult at best and, at times, impossible [see persistence…]. But the resolution between the implications of those two graphs, these two studies, isn’t a polemic difference – it’s science, a science that needs to be worked out scientifically.

As I’ve said before, I started out believing that aiming to be medication-free was the best of ideas following a psychotic episode, but my experience was different. I had believed that working together, the patient and I could identify triggers and incipient symptoms, and prevent or outrun relapses. But that was the exception rather than the rule in my [again] limited experience. In this study, they found what I saw – that the onset of relapse occurred with little or no prodrome, and the relapse itself was more than just disruptive – it was destructive. My perspective is [again] anecdotal, but theirs comes from a more scientific observational frame.

My hope would be that this report will help isolate the practical from the ideological/polemic battles that plague us. It would be [again] naive to suggest all the differences would be resolved. But this one needs to be separated and thoroughly studied. One study [left graph] says maintenance antipsychotics cause harm. The other study [graph right] says maintenance antipsychotics prevent harm. That’s the kind of difference we can only work out with careful longitudinal observations…
Mickey @ 6:00 AM

what we collectively don’t know…

Posted on Tuesday 15 December 2015

The only way to have missed hearing about this study would be to have no contact with the media in the last 24 hours. Right now, Matt Lauer is in the other  room waxing eloquent and it was the top story on my news and medical alerts this morning. Last night, the experts on the news pointed out that it was a small though significant difference and the OB types talked about the dangers of untreated depression in pregnancy:
Antidepressant Use During Pregnancy and the Risk of Autism Spectrum Disorder in Children
by Takoua Boukhris, Odile Sheehy, Laurent Mottron, and Anick Bérard
JAMA Pediatrics. 2015/. Published online December 14, 2015.

IMPORTANCE The association between the use of antidepressants during gestation and the risk of autism spectrum disorder [ASD] in children is still controversial. The etiology of ASD remains unclear, although studies have implicated genetic predispositions, environmental risk factors, and maternal depression.
OBJECTIVE To examine the risk of ASD in children associated with antidepressant use during pregnancy according to trimester of exposure and taking into account maternal depression.
DESIGN, SETTING, AND PARTICIPANTS We conducted a register-based study of an ongoing population-based cohort, the Québec Pregnancy/Children Cohort, which includes data on all pregnancies and children in Québec from January 1, 1998, to December 31, 2009. A total of 145 456 singleton full-term infants born alive and whose mothers were covered by the Régie de l’assurance maladie du Québec drug plan for at least 12 months before and during pregnancy were included. Data analysis was conducted from October 1, 2014, to June 30, 2015.
EXPOSURES Antidepressant exposure during pregnancy was defined according to trimester and specific antidepressant classes.
MAIN OUTCOMES AND MEASURES Children with ASD were defined as those with at least 1 diagnosis of ASD between date of birth and last date of follow-up. Cox proportional hazards regression models were used to estimate crude and adjusted hazard ratios with 95%CIs.
RESULTS During 904 035.50 person-years of follow-up, 1054 children [0.7%] were diagnosed with ASD; boys with ASD outnumbered girls by a ratio of about 4:1. The mean [SD] age of children at the end of follow-up was 6.24 [3.19] years. Adjusting for potential confounders, use of antidepressants during the second and/or third trimester was associated with the risk of ASD [31 exposed infants; adjusted hazard ratio, 1.87; 95%CI, 1.15-3.04]. Use of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors during the second and/or third trimester was significantly associated with an increased risk of ASD [22 exposed infants; adjusted hazard ratio, 2.17; 95%CI, 1.20-3.93]. The risk was persistent even after taking into account maternal history of depression [29 exposed infants; adjusted hazard ratio, 1.75; 95%CI, 1.03-2.97].
CONCLUSIONS AND RELEVANCE Use of antidepressants, specifically selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors, during the second and/or third trimester increases the risk of ASD in children, even after considering maternal depression. Further research is needed to specifically assess the risk of ASD associated with antidepressant types and dosages during pregnancy.


[Note: I added the bottom line – the % of their pregnancy cohort on ADs]

I generally stay away from the big population studies, mostly because I don’t have anything to say.  But his one brought back some memories. I had left the Emory full time faculty, but I heard about their doings a lot, and the use of antidepressants in pregnancy was a hot topic. It seemed like Dr. Charlie Nemeroff and Dr. Zach Stowe were everywhere talking about the dangers of untreated depression and Dr. Stowe was particularly vocal about treating depression in pregnancy. I knew a number of OBs at the time, and they all talked about how helpful he was – I think mainly because he implicitly gave them permission to use ADs [that was in a time when the promotion of antidepressants was everywhere, and patients really did show up saying, "I’ve been told I have a chemical Imbalance"]. I saw a number of depressed, pregnant women in those days [the ones who were afraid to take medications], and I can attest to the fact that depressed and pregnant is a tough place to be. Some got better and some just endured. But I thought a lot about the use of SSRIs in pregnancy. Reading was no help. Everybody had strong opinions, but they seemed morally driven rather than scientific [from both sides]. I knew I was in the nay camp myself from the start, and I remember what I said back then when  asked:
    "The most amazing undergraduate course I ever took was Embryology. We spent hours and hours looking at slides of chicken embryos, and the changes from week to week were astounding. We know next to nothing about the what drives and directs such a remarkable transformation. I was taking that course shortly after the Thalidomide tragedies, and I could see how whatever forces were operating might be easily interrupted by any number of things – including drugs. I’m on the side of staying away from drugs during pregnancy not because of what I know, but because of what we collectively don’t know."
The reason I calculated the % of patients on ADs from their data was that it looked low to me [average 1.29%]. I think the general population on ADs is definitely higher than that [though I don’t really know about Quebec from 1998 to 2009]. And that’s compatible with what I’ve heard from many pregnant women – a feeling similar to mine. So perhaps the mothers-to-be vote is often nay as well.

We worry a lot about drugs being promoted for commercial reasons. I even implied that earlier talking about Drs. Nemeroff and Stowe. But there’s another force that’s more benevolent to consider – therapeutic zeal. The OBs that I knew back then weren’t in that entrepreneurial camp at all. They were solid citizens who were genuinely worried about their patients’ plight. And I expect that the doctors and pharmacists in Germany [where Thalidomide became OTC] were using it benevolently. Hyperemesis Gravidarum can be an absolutely miserable and sometimes dangerous symptom of pregnancy, and I would bet Thalidomide was hailed as a real breakthrough [until its dreadful outcomes became apparent].

I’m aware that in this instance, I’ve gone for do no harm over therapeutic zeal, but that anything I say to explain that is post-hoc. My choice is really rooted in my reaction to those slides from 50+ years ago. It feels to me like we don’t know enough about the process of embryonic development to do anything that might mess with it and that feeling doesn’t ever change. I wonder sometimes how much these kind of personal experiences determine how we look at science, make decisions about practice, etc. And as for Autism and AD use in pregnancy, the most we can say right now is that it’s not a negative study, and the usual – awaits further study, and replication needed
Mickey @ 9:18 PM

in the land of sometimes[3]

Posted on Monday 14 December 2015

Some more statistical fluff. I don’t know the story of how the meta-analyses [study of studies] came to be and how the methodology was developed, but I know about it from the Cochrane Collaborations. Wherever and however aside, it’s pretty spectacular from where I sit. Both the metrics used and the way they’re displayed has become standardized, so a collection of many different studies can be compared and quickly understood visually. There are a lot of different techniques involved in these articles, and I’ll only cover some of the more obvious ones.. If you’re not a numbers type or you know your statistics, just skip this post. But if you want to become an amateur vetter and meta-analysis junkie, read on. After a few more posts, there will be a summary and a guide to the Internet calculators to do various aspects of the math involved. These days, meta-analyses are something a critical reader needs to know about.

In in the land of sometimes[1] and in the land of sometimes[2], I looked at some of the statistical tests used to calculate the Probabilities and Effect Sizes of continuous and categorical variables. In this installment, I want to discuss one basic format used in the meta-analyses that attempt to collate and compare multiple Clinical Trials focused on the same drug. Obviously, comparing p values wouldn’t be of much comparatave use. Probabilities are  treated as yes/no statistics determined by the prespecified alpha level [eg p < 0.05], and in spite of the temptation to say things like "very significant" or "barely made significance" based on the numeric p value, such attempts at quantification are unjustified. The obvious candidate for quantitative comparisons among studies would be the Effect Sizes. That would be comparing the strengths of some given effect across studies – which is exactly what we want to do.

So let’s first look at an example using a continuous variable comparing two different groups. The information we started with is in the left column and the values we calculated to derive the Effect Size [Cohen’s d] are in the right column [see in the land of sometimes[1]]:

Group Statistics

 
Effect Size Calculations

subjects   n1, n2 Cohen’s d    d = (μ1 – μ2) ÷ σ
means μ1, μ2 pooled std dev
std devs σ1, σ2

Remember that Cohen’s d is also known as the Standardized Mean Difference. So what we have in the right hand column is actually the distribution of the Effect Size. All we have to do now is realize that what we’ve been calling the Pooled Standard Deviation is actually the standard deviation of Cohen’s d, and that we can use it to calculate the 95% Confidence Limits using these simple formulas [either take that last part on faith or read it over several times until it makes sense]:

lower limit = d – 1.96 × σ
upper limit = d + 1.96 × σ

So now, instead of showing Cohen’s d as a frequency distribution like the upper right, we can display it more simply:

In [a], the circle represents Cohen’s d and the lines define the 95% Confidence Interval. So with a glance, you can see the Effect Size, the "spread," and whether it’s significant [the Confidence Interval line doesn’t touch or cross zero – the null hypothesis]. The example in [b] is not significant. But what I consider the brilliant part is in [c]. If you have three different studies  of a drug versus placebo, you can display them together and again, in a glance, see what’s up with all of them – the essence of meta-analysis. And there’s even more. By weighting these studies eg based on sample sizes, one can display a summary Effect Size that represents the whole collection of studies  [d]. In the case of using sample sizes:

d = (n1 × d1 + n2 × d2 + n3 × d3) ÷ (n1 + n2 + n3)

We’ve gotten so used to these forest plots that it’s easy to forget how much they’ve helped us see the big picture in one simple graphic. At least for me, this is a testimony to the adage, "a picture’s worth a thousand words." There’s something of a monotony in these clinical trials, so the information needed for this kind of comparison is almost always available. And, by the way, look at the Cochrane Collaboration logo.

Well, what about the categorical variables? One can almost say "ditto." They use the Effect Size as well, in this case the Odds Ratio. The calculation of the 95% Confidence Intervals is less intuitive, so most mortals like me  would be best advised to use one of the Internet calculators [here, here, and here]. The vertical line representing the null hypothesis is at 1 instead of 0. And there’s something else different: the 95% Confidence Interval lines are asymmetric, so authors usually use a logarithmic scale to display the values symmetrically. Look at the Odds Ratio scale on the meta-analysis of Atypical Antipsychotic augmentation I reported on earlier [creative funding III & some other things…]:

 

The land of sometimes can be like Never-Land, or Narnia, or the Land of Oz. Lots of peculiar things can and do happen in fantasy realms where not everyone knows the rules, sometimes just out of sight. Unlike the mathematics of Algebra, Calculus, Topology, etc, there’s a lot of room to wiggle and wiggle they do. Over the time I’ve been looking at these Clinical Trials, I’ve become increasingly rigid in making sure all the rules have been followed. That’s the reason for my little amateurish statistical interludes like this. I figure the more savvy the readership, the less likely it will be for the spinners of yarns to continue to have their way with us. Paradoxically, I cringe at the phrase evidence based medicine, or for that matter evidence-based-anything-else. It’s an acquired cringe, but I’ve noticed that the people who use the phrase frequently are likely using even the term itself to spin something. It’s kind of a shame. Statistical mathematics can be really elegant at times and answer questions that couldn’t be asked without it. I think of this simple methodology used in meta-analyses as an example of that…
Mickey @ 3:33 PM